28 March 2021

Terence Tao - Collected Quotes

"Algebra is what most people associate with mathematics. In a sense, this is justified. Mathematics is the study of abstract objects, numerical, logical, or geometrical, that follow a set of several carefully chosen axioms. And basic algebra is about the simplest meaningful thing that can satisfy the above definition of mathematics. There are only a dozen or so postulates, but that is enough to make the system beautifully symmetric." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"Analysis is also a heavily explored subject, and it is just as general as algebra: essentially, analysis is the study of functions and their properties. The more complicated the properties, the higher the analysis." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"Data is there to be used, so one should pick up the data and play with it. Can it produce more meaningful data?" (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"It is also a good idea to not apply any given technique or method blindly, but to think ahead and see where one could hope such a technique to take one; this can allow one to save enormous amounts of time by eliminating unprofitable directions of inquiry before sinking lots of effort into them, and conversely to give the most promising directions priority."(Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"Like and unlike the proverb above, the solution to a problem begins (and continues, and ends) with simple, logical steps. But as long as one steps in a firm, clear direction, with long strides and sharp vision, one would need far, far less than the millions of steps needed to journey a thousand miles. And mathematics, being abstract, has no physical constraints; one can always restart from scratch, try new avenues of attack, or backtrack at an instant’s notice. One does not always have these luxuries in other forms of problem-solving (e.g. trying to go home if you are lost)." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006) 

"Mathematics is a multifaceted subject, and our experience and appreciation of it changes with time and experience." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"Mathematics is sometimes thought of as great entity, like a tree, branching off into several large chunks of mathematics, which themselves branch off into specialized fields, until you reach the very ends of the tree, where you find the blossoms and the fruit. But it is not easy to classify all of mathematics into such neat compartments: there are always fuzzy regions in between branches and also extra bits outside all the classical branches." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"Mathematical problems, or puzzles, are important to real mathematics (like solving real-life problems), just as fables, stories, and anecdotes are important to the young in understanding real life. Mathematical problems are ‘sanitized’ mathematics, where an elegant solution has already been found (by someone else, of course), the question is stripped of all superfluousness and posed in an interesting and (hopefully) thought-provoking way. If mathematics is likened to prospecting for gold, solving a good mathematical problem is akin to a ‘hide-and-seek’ course in gold-prospecting: you are given a nugget to find, and you know what it looks like, that it is out there somewhere, that it is not too hard to reach, that it is unearthing within your capabilities, and you have conveniently been given the right equipment (i.e. data) to get it. It may be hidden in a cunning place, but it will require ingenuity rather than digging to reach it." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"Number theory may not necessarily be divine, but it still has an aura of mystique about it. Unlike algebra, which has as its backbone the laws of manipulating equations, number theory seems to derive its results from a source unknown." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"Understand the data. What is given in the problem? Usually, a question talks about a number of objects which satisfy some special requirements. To understand the data, one needs to see how the objects and requirements react to each other. This is important in focusing attention on the proper techniques and notation to handle the problem." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006) 

"Understand the objective. What do we want? One may need to find an object, prove a statement, determine the existence of an object with special properties, or whatever. Like the flip side of this strategy, ‘understand the data’, knowing the objective helps focus attention on the best weapons to use. Knowing the objective also helps in creating tactical goals which we know will bring us closer to solving the question." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006) 

"Write down what you know in the notation selected; draw a diagram. Putting everything down on paper helps in three ways: (a) you have an easy reference later on; (b) the paper is a good thing to stare at when you are stuck; (c) the physical act of writing down of what you know can trigger new inspirations and connections." (Terence Tao, "Solving Mathematical Problems: A Personal Perspective", 2006)

"At a purely formal level, one could call probability theory the study of measure spaces with total measure one, but that would be like calling number theory the study of strings of digits which terminate." (Terence Tao, "Topics in Random Matrix Theory", 2012)

18 March 2021

Heraclitus of Ephesus - Collected Quotes

"Everything flows and nothing stays. [...] You can't step twice into the same river." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"For those who are awake there is one common universe." (Heraclitus of Ephesus, "Fragments" [3.89], cca. 5th century)

"He who does not expect will not find out the unexpected, for it is trackless and unexplored." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"It should be known that war is universal, that strife is justice, and all things come into existence by strife and necessity." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"Much learning does not teach a man to have intelligence." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"Opposition brings concord. Out of discord comes the fairest harmony." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"The fairest order in the world is a heap of random sweepings." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"The real constitution of things is accustomed to hide itself." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"The road up and the road down is one and the same." (Heraclitus of Ephesus, "On the Universe", cca. 5th century)

"The greatest virtue is to be prudent, and wisdom is to speak the truth and with understanding to act according to nature. (Heraclitus of Ephesus, "Fragments" [75.112], cca. 5th century)

"There is one wisdom, to understand how reason steers everything through everything." (Heraclitus of Ephesus, "Fragments" [4.41], cca. 5th century)

"Time is like a river flowing endlessly through the universe." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

"Unite whole and part, agreement and disagreement, accordant and discordant; from all comes one, and from one all." (Heraclitus of Ephesus)

Ambroise-Paul-Toussaint-Jules Valéry - Collected Quotes

"The folly of mistaking a paradox for a discovery, a metaphor for a proof, a torrent of verbiage for a spring of capital truths, and oneself for an oracle, is inborn in us." (Paul Valéry, "Introduction to the Method of Leonardo da Vinci", 1895)

"La vie n'a pas le temps d'attendre la rigueur."
"Life doesn't have the time to wait for rigor." (Paul Valéry, "L'idee fixe" ["The Fix Idea"], 1932)

"Science is feasible when the variables are few and can be enumerated; when their combinations are distinct and clear. We are tending toward the condition of science and aspiring to do it. The artist works out his own formulas; the interest of science lies in the art of making science." (Paul Valéry, "Moralités" ["Morality"], 1932)

"Science means simply the aggregate of all the recipes that are always successful. All the rest is literature." (Paul Valéry, "Moralités" ["Morality"], 1932)

"The world acquires value only through its extremes and endures only through moderation; extremists make the world great, the moderates give it stability." (Paul Valéry, The Nation, 1957)

"All our language is composed of brief little dreams; and the wonderful thing is that we sometimes make of them strangely accurate and marvelously reasonable thoughts. […] What should we be without the help of that which does not exist? Very little. And our unoccupied minds would languish if fables, mistaken notions, abstractions, beliefs, and monsters, hypotheses, and the so-called problems of metaphysics did not people with beings and objectless images our natural depths and darkness. Myths are the souls of our actions and our loves. We cannot act without moving towards a phantom. We can love only what we create." (Paul Valéry, "The Outlook for Intelligence", 1962)

"The machine rules. Human life is rigorously controlled by it, dominated by the terribly precise will of mechanisms. These creatures of man are exacting. They are now reacting on their creators, making them like themselves. They want well-trained humans; they are gradually wiping out the differences between men, fitting them into their own orderly functioning, into the uniformity of their own regimes. They are thus shaping humanity for their own use, almost in their own image." (Paul A Valéry, "Fairy Tales for Computers", 1969)

"Small unexplained facts always contain grounds for upsetting all explanations of 'big' facts." (Paul Valéry)

"Space is an imaginary body, as time is fictive movement. When we say 'in space' or 'space is filled with' we are positing a body." (Paul Valéry)

"The universe is built on a plan the profound symmetry of which is somehow present in the inner structure of our intellect." (Paul Valéry)

Courtney Brown - Collected Quotes

"As with subtle bifurcations, catastrophes also involve a control parameter. When the value of that parameter is below a bifurcation point, the system is dominated by one attractor. When the value of that parameter is above the bifurcation point, another attractor dominates. Thus the fundamental characteristic of a catastrophe is the sudden disappearance of one attractor and its basin, combined with the dominant emergence of another attractor. Any type of attractor static, periodic, or chaotic can be involved in this. Elementary catastrophe theory involves static attractors, such as points. Because multidimensional surfaces can also attract (together with attracting points on these surfaces), we refer to them more generally as attracting hypersurfaces, limit sets, or simply attractors." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"Chaos and catastrophe theories are among the most interesting recent developments in nonlinear modeling, and both have captured the interests of scientists in many disciplines. It is only natural that social scientists should be concerned with these theories. Linear statistical models have proven very useful in a great deal of social scientific empirical analyses, as is evidenced by how widely these models have been used for a number of decades. However, there is no apparent reason, intuitive or otherwise, as to why human behavior should be more linear than the behavior of other things, living and nonliving. Thus an intellectual movement toward nonlinear models is an appropriate evolutionary movement in social scientific thinking, if for no other reason than to expand our paradigmatic boundaries by encouraging greater flexibility in our algebraic specifications of all aspects of human life." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"[...] chaos and catastrophe theories per se address behavioral phenomena that are consequences of two general types of nonlinear dynamic behavior. In the most elementary of behavioral terms, chaotic phenomena are a class of deterministic processes that seem to mimic random or stochastic dynamics. Catastrophe phenomena, on the other hand, are a class of dynamic processes that exhibit a sudden and large scale change in at least one variable in correspondence with relatively small changes in other variables or, in some cases, parameters." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"Chaos and catastrophe theories directly address the social scientists' need to understand classes of nonlinear complexities that are certain to appear in social phenomena. The probabilistic properties of many chaos and catastrophe models are simply not known, and there is little likelihood that general procedures will be developed soon to alleviate the difficulties inherent with probabilistic approaches in such complicated settings." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"Chaos has three fundamental characteristics. They are (a) irregular periodicity, (b) sensitivity to initial conditions, and (c) a lack of predictability. These characteristics interact within any one chaotic setting to produce highly complex nonlinear variable trajectories." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"Fundamental to catastrophe theory is the idea of a bifurcation. A bifurcation is an event that occurs in the evolution of a dynamic system in which the characteristic behavior of the system is transformed. This occurs when an attractor in the system changes in response to change in the value of a parameter. A catastrophe is one type of bifurcation. The broader framework within which catastrophes are located is called dynamical bifurcation theory." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"In addition to dimensionality requirements, chaos can occur only in nonlinear situations. In multidimensional settings, this means that at least one term in one equation must be nonlinear while also involving several of the variables. With all linear models, solutions can be expressed as combinations of regular and linear periodic processes, but nonlinearities in a model allow for instabilities in such periodic solutions within certain value ranges for some of the parameters." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"In classical catastrophe theory, the various attracting static hypersurfaces are actually connected. However, there are portions of the overall surface that are unstable, and thus repelling. Thus nearby trajectories tend to 'fly' quickly past these unstable regions as they move from one stable area to another. It is this relatively rapid snapping movement that is typical of nearly all catastrophe phenomena." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"In its essence, chaos is an irregular oscillatory process. Because chaos is a subset of the more general classification of oscillatory dynamics, it is useful before venturing into chaos to review briefly the extent to which regular oscillatory processes influence human behavior." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"Small changes in the initial conditions in a chaotic system produce dramatically different evolutionary histories. It is because of this sensitivity to initial conditions that chaotic systems are inherently unpredictable. To predict a future state of a system, one has to be able to rely on numerical calculations and initial measurements of the state variables. Yet slight errors in measurement combined with extremely small computational errors (from roundoff or truncation) make prediction impossible from a practical perspective. Moreover, small initial errors in prediction grow exponentially in chaotic systems as the trajectories evolve. Thus, theoretically, prediction may be possible with some chaotic processes if one is interested only in the movement between two relatively close points on a trajectory. When longer time intervals are involved, the situation becomes hopeless."(Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"The dimensionality and nonlinearity requirements of chaos do not guarantee its appearance. At best, these conditions allow it to occur, and even then under limited conditions relating to particular parameter values. But this does not imply that chaos is rare in the real world. Indeed, discoveries are being made constantly of either the clearly identifiable or arguably persuasive appearance of chaos. Most of these discoveries are being made with regard to physical systems, but the lack of similar discoveries involving human behavior is almost certainly due to the still developing nature of nonlinear analyses in the social sciences rather than the absence of chaos in the human setting."  (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"Thus my advice to model builders in the social sciences is to think in terms of social processes that might require algebraic structures that could yield catastrophe potential. Build models from an intimate knowledge of these processes while remaining aware of the algebraic requirements for catastrophes. The art of nonlinear model building is a delicate dance with two partners, algebraic forms that produce known effects and a substantive understanding of the complexities of social phenomena. Coordinating the two by mixing structure to match complexity is the job of the theorist, and it is the single greatest creative challenge of any researcher." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

On Chaos IV

"One of the central problems studied by mankind is the problem of the succession of form. Whatever is the ultimate nature of reality (assuming that this expression has meaning), it is indisputable that our universe is not chaos. We perceive beings, objects, things to which we give names. These beings or things are forms or structures endowed with a degree of stability; they take up some part of space and last for some period of time." (René Thom, "Structural Stability and Morphogenesis", 1972)

"'Disorder' is not mere chaos; it implies defective order." (John M Ziman, "Models of Disorder", 1979)

"Chaos and catastrophe theories are among the most interesting recent developments in nonlinear modeling, and both have captured the interests of scientists in many disciplines. It is only natural that social scientists should be concerned with these theories. Linear statistical models have proven very useful in a great deal of social scientific empirical analyses, as is evidenced by how widely these models have been used for a number of decades. However, there is no apparent reason, intuitive or otherwise, as to why human behavior should be more linear than the behavior of other things, living and nonliving. Thus an intellectual movement toward nonlinear models is an appropriate evolutionary movement in social scientific thinking, if for no other reason than to expand our paradigmatic boundaries by encouraging greater flexibility in our algebraic specifications of all aspects of human life." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"[...] chaos and catastrophe theories per se address behavioral phenomena that are consequences of two general types of nonlinear dynamic behavior. In the most elementary of behavioral terms, chaotic phenomena are a class of deterministic processes that seem to mimic random or stochastic dynamics. Catastrophe phenomena, on the other hand, are a class of dynamic processes that exhibit a sudden and large scale change in at least one variable in correspondence with relatively small changes in other variables or, in some cases, parameters." (Courtney Brown, "Chaos and Catastrophe Theories", 1995)

"Nature normally hates power laws. In ordinary systems all quantities follow bell curves, and correlations decay rapidly, obeying exponential laws. But all that changes if the system is forced to undergo a phase transition. Then power laws emerge-nature's unmistakable sign that chaos is departing in favor of order. The theory of phase transitions told us loud and clear that the road from disorder to order is maintained by the powerful forces of self-organization and is paved by power laws. It told us that power laws are not just another way of characterizing a system's behavior. They are the patent signatures of self-organization in complex systems." (Albert-László Barabási, "Linked: How Everything Is Connected to Everything Else and What It Means for Business, Science, and Everyday Life", 2002)

"Chaos is not pure disorder, it carries within itself the indistinctness between the potentialities of order, of disorder, and of organization from which a cosmos will be born, which is an ordered universe." (Edgar Morin, "Restricted Complexity, General Complexity" [in (Carlos Gershenson et al [Eds.], "Worldviews, Science and Us: Philosophy and Complexity", 2007)])

"Chaos can be understood as a dynamical process in which microscopic information hidden in the details of a system’s state is dug out and expanded to a macroscopically visible scale (stretching), while the macroscopic information visible in the current system’s state is continuously discarded (folding)." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

"God has put a secret art into the forces of Nature so as to enable it to fashion itself out of chaos into a perfect world system." (Immanuel Kant)

"Science, like art, music and poetry, tries to reduce chaos to the clarity and order of pure beauty." (Detlev W Bronk)

17 March 2021

Catastrophe Theory II

"What I am offering, is not a scientific theory, but a method; the first step in the construction of a model is to describe the dynamical models compatible with an empirically given morphology, and this is also the first step in understanding the phenomena under consideration. [...] We may hope that theoreticians will develop a quantitative model [for specific processes described by catastrophe theory ...] But this is only a hope." (René Thom, "Structural Stability and Morphogenesis", 1972)

"The catastrophe model is at the same time much less and much more than a scientific theory; one should consider it as a language, a method, which permits classification and systematization of given empirical data [...] In fact, any phenomenon at all can be explained by a suitable model from catastrophe theory." (René F Thom, 1973)

"First, nature's line patterns are not all of the same sort; the triple junctions generic in mud cracks cannot occur with caustics. Second, the geometrical optics of cylindrically symmetric artifacts such as telescopes, where departures from the ideal point focus are treated as 'aberrations', is very different from the geometrical optics of nature, where the generic forms of caustic surfaces are governed by the mathematics of catastrophe theory." Michael V Berry & John F Nye, "Fine Structure in Caustic Junctions", Nature Vol. 267 (3606), 1977)

"the claims made for the theory are greatly exaggerated and its accomplishments, at least in the biological and social sciences, are insignificant. [...] Catastrophe theory is one of many attempts that have been made to deduce the world by thought alone [...] an appealing dream for mathematicians, but a dream that cannot come true."  (Héctor J Sussmann & Raphael S Zahler, Nature, 1977)

"A catastrophe, in the very broad sense [René] Thom gives to the word, is any discontinuous transition that occurs when a system can have more than one stable state, or can follow more than one stable pathway of change. The catastrophe is the 'jump' from one state or pathway to another." (Alexander Woodcock & Monte Davis, "Catastrophe Theory", 1978)

"It is not enough to know the critical stress, that is, the quantitative breaking point of a complex design; one should also know as much as possible of the qualitative geometry of its failure modes, because what will happen beyond the critical stress level can be very different from one case to the next, depending on just which path the buckling takes. And here catastrophe theory, joined with bifurcation theory, can be very helpful by indicating how new failure modes appear." (Alexander Woodcock & Monte Davis, "Catastrophe Theory", 1978)

"The unfoldings are called catastrophes because each of them has regions where a dynamic system can jump suddenly from one state to another, although the factors controlling the process change continuously. Each of the seven catastrophes represents a pattern of behavior determined only by the number of control factors, not by their nature or by the interior mechanisms that connect them to the system's behavior. Therefore, the elementary catastrophes can be models for a wide variety of processes, even those in which we know little about the quantitative laws involved." (Alexander Woodcock & Monte Davis, "Catastrophe Theory", 1978)

"Two assumptions are needed to apply catastrophe theory as it now stands: first, that the system described be governed by a potential, and second, that its behavior depend on a limited number of control factors. Without these assumptions, the classification of the elementary catastrophes is impossible." (Alexander Woodcock & Monte Davis, "Catastrophe Theory", 1978)

"It is more a philosophy than mathematics, and even as a philosophy it doesn't explain the real world [...] as mathematics, it brings together two of the most basic ideas in modern math: the study of dynamic systems and the study of the singularities of maps. Together, they cover a very wide area - but catastrophe theory brings them together in an arbitrary and constrained way." (Steven Smale)

"While it must be granted that a number of immoderate claims in the form of 'catastrophe theory can do everything' have been made in the literature, on the basis of too little experience, it doesn't seem that the proper response is an equally immoderate claim that 'catastrophe theory can do nothing' on the basis of that same body of experience." (Robert Rosen)

Catastrophe Theory I

"[...] if the behavior points for the entire control surface are plotted and then connected, they form a smooth surface: the behavior surface. The surface has an overall slope from high values where rage predominates to low values in the region where fear is the prevailing state of mind, but the slope is not its most distinctive feature. Catastrophe theory reveals that in the middle of the surface there must be a smooth double fold, creating a pleat without creases, which grows narrower from the front of the surface to the back and eventually disappears in a singular point where the three sheets of the pleat come together. It is the pleat that gives the model its most interesting characteristics. All the points on the behavior surface represent the most probable behavior [...], with the exception of those on the middle sheet, which represent least probable behavior. Through catastrophe theory we can deduce the shape of the entire surface from the fact that the behavior is bimodal for some control points." (E Cristopher Zeeman, "Catastrophe Theory", Scientific American, 1976)

"Catastrophe Theory is - quite likely - the first coherent attempt (since Aristotelian logic) to give a theory on analogy. When narrow-minded scientists object to Catastrophe Theory that it gives no more than analogies, or metaphors, they do not realise that they are stating the proper aim of Catastrophe Theory, which is to classify all possible types of analogous situations." (René F Thom," La Théorie des catastrophes: État présent et perspective", 1977)

"'Catastrophe theory' denotes both a purely mathematical discipline describing certain singularities of smooth maps, as well as the concerted effort to apply these theorems to a wide variety of problems in fields ranging from linguistics and psychology to embryology, evolution, physics, and engineering." (Héctor J Sussmann & Raphael S Zahler, "Catastrophe Theory as Applied to the Social and Biological Sciences: A Critique" Synthese Vol. 37 (2), 1978)

"Because of its foundation in topology, catastrophe theory is qualitative, not quantitative. Just as geometry treated the properties of a triangle without regard to its size, so topology deals with properties that have no magnitude, for example, the property of a given point being inside or outside a closed curve or surface. This property is what topologists call 'invariant' -it does not change even when the curve is distorted. A topologist may work with seven-dimensional space, but he does not and cannot measure (in the ordinary sense) along any of those dimensions. The ability to classify and manipulate all types of form is achieved only by giving up concepts such as size, distance, and rate. So while catastrophe theory is well suited to describe and even to predict the shape of processes, its descriptions and predictions are not quantitative like those of theories built upon calculus. Instead, they are rather like maps without a scale: they tell us that there are mountains to the left, a river to the right, and a cliff somewhere ahead, but not how far away each is, or how large." (Alexander Woodcock & Monte Davis, "Catastrophe Theory", 1978)

"But there is another kind of change, too, change that is less suited to mathematical analysis: the abrupt bursting of a bubble, the discontinuous transition from ice at its melting point to water at its freezing point, the qualitative shift in our minds when we 'get' a pun or a play on words. Catastrophe theory is a mathematical language created to describe and classify this second type of change. It challenges scientists to change the way they think about processes and events in many fields." (Alexander Woodcock & Monte Davis, "Catastrophe Theory", 1978)

"Catastrophe theory is a controversial new way of thinking about change - change in a course of events, change in an object's shape, change in a system's behavior, change in ideas themselves. Its name suggests disaster, and indeed the theory can be applied to literal catastrophes such as the collapse of a bridge or the downfall of an empire. But it also deals with changes as quiet as the dancing of sunlight on the bottom of a pool of water and as subtle as the transition from waking to sleep." (Alexander Woodcock & Monte Davis, "Catastrophe Theory", 1978)

"Catastrophes are often stimulated by the failure to feel the emergence of a domain, and so what cannot be felt in the imagination is experienced as embodied sensation in the catastrophe. (William I Thompson, "Gaia, a Way of Knowing: Political Implications of the New Biology", 1987)

"A catastrophe is a universal unfolding of a singular function germ. The singular function germs are called organization centers of the catastrophes. [...] Catastrophe theory is concerned with the mathematical modeling of sudden changes - so called 'catastrophes' - in the behavior of natural systems, which can appear as a consequence of continuous changes of the system parameters. While in common speech the word catastrophe has a negative connotation, in mathematics it is neutral." (Werner Sanns, "Catastrophe Theory" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

"Catastrophe theory can be thought of as a link between classical analysis, dynamical systems, differential topology (including singularity theory), modern bifurcation theory and the theory of complex systems. [...] The name ‘catastrophe theory’ is used for a combination of singularity theory and its applications. [...] From the didactical point of view, there are two main positions for courses in catastrophe theory at university level: Trying to teach the theory as a perfect axiomatic system consisting of exact definitions, theorems and proofs or trying to teach mathematics as it can be developed from historical or from natural problems." (Werner Sanns, "Catastrophe Theory" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

"Classification is only one of the mathematical aspects of catastrophe theory. Another is stability. The stable states of natural systems are the ones that we can observe over a longer period of time. But the stable states of a system, which can be described by potential functions and their singularities, can become unstable if the potentials are changed by perturbations. So stability problems in nature lead to mathematical questions concerning the stability of the potential functions." (Werner Sanns, "Catastrophe Theory" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

Hiroki Sayama - Collected Quotes

"A model is a simplified representation of a system. It can be conceptual, verbal, diagrammatic, physical, or formal (mathematical)." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

"A network (or graph) consists of a set of nodes (or vertices, actors) and a set of edges (or links, ties) that connect those nodes. [...] The size of a network is characterized by the numbers of nodes and edges in it." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

"Chaos can be understood as a dynamical process in which microscopic information hidden in the details of a system’s state is dug out and expanded to a macroscopically visible scale (stretching), while the macroscopic information visible in the current system’s state is continuously discarded (folding)." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

"Complex systems are networks made of a number of components that interact with each other, typically in a nonlinear fashion. Complex systems may arise and evolve through self-organization, such that they are neither completely regular nor completely random, permitting the development of emergent behavior at macroscopic scales." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

"Dynamics of a linear system are decomposable into multiple independent one-dimensional exponential dynamics, each of which takes place along the direction given by an eigenvector. A general trajectory from an arbitrary initial condition can be obtained by a simple linear superposition of those independent dynamics." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

"Emergence is a nontrivial relationship between the properties of a system at microscopic and macroscopic scales. Macroscopic properties are called emergent when it is hard to explain them simply from microscopic properties." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

"Self-organization is a dynamical process by which a system spontaneously forms nontrivial macroscopic structures and/or behaviors over time." (Hiroki Sayama, "Introduction to the Modeling and Analysis of Complex Systems", 2015)

Mathematical Models III

"Mathematical model making is an art. If the model is too small, a great deal of analysis and numerical solution can be done, but the results, in general, can be meaningless. If the model is too large, neither analysis nor numerical solution can be carried out, the interpretation of the results is in any case very difficult, and there is great difficulty in obtaining the numerical values of the parameters needed for numerical results." (Richard E Bellman, "Eye of the Hurricane: An Autobiography", 1984)

"Symmetries abound in nature, in technology, and - especially - in the simplified mathematical models we study so assiduously. Symmetries complicate things and simplify them. They complicate them by introducing exceptional types of behavior, increasing the number of variables involved, and making vanish things that usually do not vanish. They simplify them by introducing exceptional types of behavior, increasing the number of variables involved, and making vanish things that usually do not vanish. They violate all the hypotheses of our favorite theorems, yet lead to natural generalizations of those theorems. It is now standard to study the 'generic' behavior of dynamical systems. Symmetry is not generic. The answer is to work within the world of symmetric systems and to examine a suitably restricted idea of genericity." (Ian Stewart, "Bifurcation with symmetry", 1988)

"Pedantry and sectarianism aside, the aim of theoretical physics is to construct mathematical models such as to enable us, from the use of knowledge gathered in a few observations, to predict by logical processes the outcomes in many other circumstances. Any logically sound theory satisfying this condition is a good theory, whether or not it be derived from ‘ultimate’ or ‘fundamental’ truth." (Clifford Truesdell & Walter Noll, "The Non-Linear Field Theories of Mechanics" 2nd Ed., 1992)

"[…] interval mathematics and fuzzy logic together can provide a promising alternative to mathematical modeling for many physical systems that are too vague or too complicated to be described by simple and crisp mathematical formulas or equations. When interval mathematics and fuzzy logic are employed, the interval of confidence and the fuzzy membership functions are used as approximation measures, leading to the so-called fuzzy systems modeling." (Guanrong Chen & Trung Tat Pham, "Introduction to Fuzzy Sets, Fuzzy Logic, and Fuzzy Control Systems", 2001)

"Modeling, in a general sense, refers to the establishment of a description of a system (a plant, a process, etc.) in mathematical terms, which characterizes the input-output behavior of the underlying system. To describe a physical system […] we have to use a mathematical formula or equation that can represent the system both qualitatively and quantitatively. Such a formulation is a mathematical representation, called a mathematical model, of the physical system." (Guanrong Chen & Trung Tat Pham, "Introduction to Fuzzy Sets, Fuzzy Logic, and Fuzzy Control Systems", 2001)

"An important aspect of the global theory of dynamical systems is the stability of the orbit structure as a whole. The motivation for the corresponding theory comes from applied mathematics. Mathematical models always contain simplifying assumptions. Dominant features are modeled; supposed small disturbing forces are ignored. Thus, it is natural to ask if the qualitative structure of the set of solutions - the phase portrait - of a model would remain the same if small perturbations were included in the model. The corresponding mathematical theory is called structural stability." (Carmen Chicone, "Stability Theory of Ordinary Differential Equations" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

"Models do not and need not match reality in all of its aspects and details to be adequate. A mathematical model is usually developed for a specific class of target systems, and its validity is determined relative to its intended applications. A model is considered valid within its intended domain of applicability provided that its predictions in that domain fall within an acceptable range of error, specified prior to the model’s development or identification." (Zoltan Domotor, "Mathematical Models in Philosophy of Science" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

"Simplified description of a real world system in mathematical terms, e. g., by means of differential equations or other suitable mathematical structures." (Benedetto Piccoli, Andrea Tosin, "Vehicular Traffic: A Review of Continuum Mathematical Models" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

"Stated loosely, models are simplified, idealized and approximate representations of the structure, mechanism and behavior of real-world systems. From the standpoint of set-theoretic model theory, a mathematical model of a target system is specified by a nonempty set - called the model’s domain, endowed with some operations and relations, delineated by suitable axioms and intended empirical interpretation." (Zoltan Domotor, "Mathematical Models in Philosophy of Science" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

"The standard view among most theoretical physicists, engineers and economists is that mathematical models are syntactic (linguistic) items, identified with particular systems of equations or relational statements. From this perspective, the process of solving a designated system of (algebraic, difference, differential, stochastic, etc.) equations of the target system, and interpreting the particular solutions directly in the context of predictions and explanations are primary, while the mathematical structures of associated state and orbit spaces, and quantity algebras – although conceptually important, are secondary." (Zoltan Domotor, "Mathematical Models in Philosophy of Science" [Mathematics of Complexity and Dynamical Systems, 2012])

13 March 2021

George Siemens - Collected Quotes

"An ecology provides the special formations needed by organizations. Ecologies are: loose, free, dynamic, adaptable, messy, and chaotic. Innovation does not arise through hierarchies. As a function of creativity, innovation requires trust, openness, and a spirit of experimentation - where random ideas and thoughts can collide for re-creation." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Change pressures arise from different sectors of a system. At times it is mandated from the top of a hierarchy, other times it forms from participants at a grass-roots level. Some changes are absorbed by the organization without significant impact on, or alterations of, existing methods. In other cases, change takes root. It causes the formation of new methods (how things are done and what is possible) within the organization." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Complexity and diversity results in specialized nodes (a single entity can no longer know all required elements). The act of knowledge growth and learning involves connected specialized nodes." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Connections create structures. Structures do not create (though they may facilitate) connections. Our approaches today reflect this error in thinking. We have tried to do the wrong thing first with knowledge. We determine that we will have a certification before we determine what it is that we want to certify. We need to enable the growth of connections and observe the structures that emerge." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Context is not as simple as being in a different space [...] context includes elements like our emotions, recent experiences, beliefs, and the surrounding environment - each element possesses attributes, that when considered in a certain light, informs what is possible in the discussion." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Knowledge flow can be likened to a river that meanders through the ecology of an organization. In certain areas, the river pools and in other areas it ebbs. The health of the learning ecology of the organization depends on effective nurturing of flow." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Learning is a multi-faceted, integrated process where changes with any one element alters the larger network. Knowledge is subject to the nuances of complex, adaptive systems." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Hierarchy adapts knowledge to the organization; a network adapts the organization to the knowledge." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Learning is the process of creating networks. Nodes are external entities which we can use to form a network. Or nodes may be people, organizations, libraries, web sites, books, journals, database, or any other source of information. The act of learning (things become a bit tricky here) is one of creating an external network of nodes - where we connect and form information and knowledge sources. The learning that happens in our heads is an internal network (neural). Learning networks can then be perceived as structures that we create in order to stay current and continually acquire, experience, create, and connect new knowledge (external). And learning networks can be perceived as structures that exist within our minds (internal) in connecting and creating patterns of understanding." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Nodes and connectors comprise the structure of a network. In contrast, an ecology is a living organism. It influences the formation of the network itself." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"Our pre-conceived structures of interpreting knowledge sometimes interfere with new knowledge." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

"When we focus on designing ecologies in which people can forage for knowledge, we are less concerned about communicating the minutiae of changing knowledge. Instead, we are creating the conduit through which knowledge will flow." (George Siemens, "Knowing Knowledge", 2006)

12 March 2021

Diego Rasskin-Gutman - Collected Quotes

"A chess hypothesis is basically the equivalent to drawing up a strategic plan. Experimentation in chess is equivalent to the moves that are found to carry out each plan. Throughout the history of chess, both the plans (the hypotheses) as well as the moves (the experiments) have been evolving (thanks to results from the practice of the game and from analyses), and this knowledge is the patrimony of professional players." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"A second class of metaphors - mathematical algorithms, heuristics, and models - brings us closer to the world of computer science programs, simulations, and approximations of the brain and its cognitive processes." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"An algorithm refers to a successive and finite procedure by which it is possible to solve a certain problem. Algorithms are the operational base for most computer programs. They consist of a series of instructions that, thanks to programmers’ prior knowledge about the essential characteristics of a problem that must be solved, allow a step-by-step path to the solution." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Any scientific hypothesis springs from knowledge that was previously generated by observations of facts in the real world. In addition, hypotheses produce predictions that need to be tested. For some, scientific definitions are limited to natural phenomena (although this definition would require mathematics to stop being a science since it deals with ideal objects)." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"As art, chess speaks to us of the personal decisions that are made in the course of a game. Looking at this facet of the game, the essential protagonist is the aesthetic sense rather than the capacity for calculation, which thus moves us closer to the human dimension and farther from mathematical algorithms." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Chess, as a game of zero sum and total information is, theoretically, a game that can be solved. The problem is the immensity of the search tree: the total number of positions surpasses the number of atoms in our galaxy. When there are few pieces on the board, the search space is greatly reduced, and the problem becomes trivial for computers’ calculation capacity." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Chess also offers a modality that includes an exercise of totally free creation - compositions. These artificial positions are created for didactic reasons to illustrate a certain subject or to propose a problem that has to be solved following a series of indications" (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Chess is human communication. Each player, in each move, must understand the opponent’s message or soon fall into difficulties. In this way, the creative act is united with the capacity to understand the opponent’s intentions, resulting in a fight of ideas, wills, and creative imagination." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Cognitive psychology has followed a different direction, considering intelligence as a set of mental representations and a series of processes that operate on these representations that allows the individual to adapt to the changing conditions of the environment. This type of approach is connected with information theory. The intelligent mind operates by processing information that it collects from the environment, and the better and faster this information is processed, the more intelligence is demonstrated." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Finally, chess has a science - like special attraction since it lets the player first propose hypotheses of different strategic plans that are based on the game rules and possible moves of the pieces and then refute those hypotheses after careful investigation of the different lines of play. This process is analogous to the everyday work of a scientist." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"From its mystical origins as a dialogue with the supernatural powers to a metaphor for war, chess passes through a period as a representation of order in the universe until it becomes the game-art-science that millions of people all over the world are passionate about and that has developed into a testing ground for the sciences of artificial intelligence and cognitive psychology." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Game theory postulates rational behavior for each participant. Each player is conscious of the rules and behaves in accordance with them, each player has sufficient knowledge of the situation in which he or she is involved to be able to evaluate what the best option is when it comes to taking action (a move), and each player takes into account the decisions that might be made by other participants and their repercussions with respect to his or her own decision. Game theory about zero-sum games with two participants is relevant for chess. In this type of situation, each action that is favorable to one participant (player) is proportionally unfavorable for the opponent. Thus, the gain of one represents the loss of the other." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Game theory proposes a method called minimization-maximization (minimax) that determines the best possibility that is available to a player by following a decision tree that minimizes the opponent’s gain and maximizes the player’s own. This important algorithm is the basis for generating algorithms for chess programs." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"If intelligence is a capacity that is gradually acquired as a result of development and learning, then a machine that can learn from experience would have, at least in theory, the capacity to carry out intelligent behavior. [...] Humans have created machines that imitate us - that provide mirrors to see ourselves and measure our strength, our intellect, and even our creativity." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"In emergent processes, the whole is greater than the sum of the parts. A mathematical phenomenon that appears in certain dynamic systems also occurs within biological systems, from molecular interactions within the cells to the cognitive processes that we use to move within society. [...] Emergent patterns of ideas, beauty, desires, or tragicomedy wait, ready to trap the next traveler in their complex domain of neatly patterned squares - the never-ending world of chess metaphors." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"In fact, H [entropy] measures the amount of uncertainty that exists in the phenomenon. If there were only one event, its probability would be equal to 1, and H would be equal to 0 - that is, there is no uncertainty about what will happen in a phenomenon with a single event because we always know what is going to occur. The more events that a phenomenon possesses, the more uncertainty there is about the state of the phenomenon. In other words, the more entropy, the more information." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Many terms that are used to comment on games are aesthetic allusions, indicating that among chess players it is hard to separate out the game’s creative and analytic aspects. Terms that are frequently used include subtlety, depth, beauty, surprise, vision, brilliance, elegance, harmony, and symmetry." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"On the surface, chess is a game that has a winner and a loser. However, a deeper look reveals that perhaps chess is not just a game but a line of communication between two brains. [...] chess is a communication device. As with any other act of communication, it is necessary to have someone who sends the message, a transmission medium, and someone who receives the message. Players are both the communicators and receivers; the board and the chess pieces are the transmission medium. In an exchange of messages, ideas, attitudes, and personal positions about the uncertainty of our world, however, where is the win, and where is the loss?" (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Pattern perception (that is, the perception of similarities in spatial or temporal configurations) has a fundamental role in playing chess [...] The two essential components in decision making in chess are recognizing patterns stored in long-term memory (which requires an exhaustive knowledge database) and searching for a solution within the problem space. The first component uses perception and long-term memory, and the second leans mainly on the calculation of variations, which in turn has its foundations in logical reasoning." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The brain and its cognitive mental processes are the biological foundation for creating metaphors about the world and oneself. Artificial intelligence, human beings’ attempt to transcend their biology, tries to enter into these scenarios to learn how they function. But there is another metaphor of the world that has its own particular landscapes, inhabitants, and laws. The brain provides the organic structure that is necessary for generating the mind, which in turn is considered a process that results from brain activity." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The master of chess is deeply familiar with these patterns and knows very well the position that would be beneficial to reach. The rest is thinking in a logical way (calculating) about how each piece should be moved to reach the new pattern that has already taken shape in the chess player’s mind. This way of facing chess is closely related to the solving of theorems in mathematics. For example, a mathematician who wishes to prove an equation needs to imagine how the terms on each side of the equal sign can be manipulated so that one is reduced to the other. The enterprise is far from easy, to judge by the more than two hundred years that have been needed to solve theorems such as that of Fermat (z^n = x^n + y^n), using diverse tricks to prove the equation." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The mind creates a metaphor of ourselves and of the world that surrounds us. And it is so skillful that it has created machines that are capable of simulating human beings’ own creativity in a series of 1s and 0s [...]" (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The complexities of the universe are reflected in the complexities of our brains and in that natural, intimate and solitary activity that we call mind. In this process of matching up and representing, the inexhaustible human curiosity accepts the ancestral challenge of exploring the enormity of what we have yet to know. Chess, a world of fixed rules but with almost infinite borders, is an approachable model of that profound and endless human search." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The problem of identifying the subset of good moves is much more complicated than simply counting the total number of possibilities and falls completely into the domain of strategy and tactics of chess as a game." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The simplest basic architecture of an artificial neural network is composed of three layers of neurons - input, output, and intermediary (historically called perceptron). When the input layer is stimulated, each node responds in a particular way by sending information to the intermediary level nodes, which in turn distribute it to the output layer nodes and thereby generate a response. The key to artificial neural networks is in the ways that the nodes are connected and how each node reacts to the stimuli coming from the nodes it is connected to. Just as with the architecture of the brain, the nodes allow information to pass only if a specific stimulus threshold is passed. This threshold is governed by a mathematical equation that can take different forms. The response depends on the sum of the stimuli coming from the input node connections and is 'all or nothing'." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Vision is a capacity to understand a position and to generate solid strategic plans. And a good base of chess knowledge is needed to understand what it means to play with brilliance or elegance." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"With Kurt Gödel, we fi nd in the twentieth century the idea that formal systems are incomplete, a concept that is perhaps important to chess theory. If undecidable statements exist in chess, then it is impossible to solve them completely with a computer chess program." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

On Chess VI: Trivia III

"The chemists who uphold dualism are far from being agreed among themselves; nevertheless, all of them in maintaining their opinion, rely upon the phenomena of chemical reactions. For a long time the uncertainty of this method has been pointed out: it has been shown repeatedly, that the atoms put into movement during a reaction take at that time a new arrangement, and that it is impossible to deduce the old arrangement from the new one. It is as if, in the middle of a game of chess, after the disarrangement of all the pieces, one of the players should wish, from the inspection of the new place occupied by each piece, to determine that which it originally occupied." (Auguste Laurent, "Chemical Method", 1855)

"And as the number of combinations that can be made on the chess-board, is so great that probably no two games exactly alike were ever played; so no two games which the student plays with nature to wrest from her hidden truths, which were worth playing at all, ever made use of quite the same methods in quite the same way." (Alfred Marshall, "Principles of Economics", 1890)

"Imagine that [...] the world is something like a great chess game being played by the gods, and we are observers of the game. [...] If we watch long enough, we may eventually catch on to a few of the rules [...]. However, we might not be able to understand why a particular move is made in the game, merely because it is too complicated and our minds are limited [...]. We must limit ourselves to the more basic question of the rules of the game. If we know the rules, we consider that we 'understand' the world." (Richard P. Feynman, "The Feynman Lectures on Physics", 1964)

"If arithmetical skill is the measure of intelligence, then computers have been more intelligent than all human beings all along. If the ability to play chess is the measure, then there are computers now in existence that are more intelligent than any but a very few human beings. However, if insight, intuition, creativity, the ability to view a problem as a whole and guess the answer by the “feel” of the situation, is a measure of intelligence, computers are very unintelligent indeed. Nor can we see right now how this deficiency in computers can be easily remedied, since human beings cannot program a computer to be intuitive or creative for the very good reason that we do not know what we ourselves do when we exercise these qualities." (Isaac Asimov, "Machines That Think", 1983)

"Chess is a unique cognitive nexus, a place where art and science come together in the human mind and are then refined and improved by experience." (Garry Kasparov, "How Life Imitates Chess: Making the Right Moves, from the Board to the Boardroom", 2007)

"Finally, chess has a science - like special attraction since it lets the player first propose hypotheses of different strategic plans that are based on the game rules and possible moves of the pieces and then refute those hypotheses after careful investigation of the different lines of play. This process is analogous to the everyday work of a scientist." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Game theory postulates rational behavior for each participant. Each player is conscious of the rules and behaves in accordance with them, each player has sufficient knowledge of the situation in which he or she is involved to be able to evaluate what the best option is when it comes to taking action (a move), and each player takes into account the decisions that might be made by other participants and their repercussions with respect to his or her own decision. Game theory about zero-sum games with two participants is relevant for chess. In this type of situation, each action that is favorable to one participant (player) is proportionally unfavorable for the opponent. Thus, the gain of one represents the loss of the other." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The complexities of the universe are reflected in the complexities of our brains and in that natural, intimate and solitary activity that we call mind. In this process of matching up and representing, the inexhaustible human curiosity accepts the ancestral challenge of exploring the enormity of what we have yet to know. Chess, a world of fixed rules but with almost infinite borders, is an approachable model of that profound and endless human search." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Playing chess involves following the rules of the game (ingenuity), and it also seems to require insight (intuition) into which rules to choose given different positions on the game board. To win at chess, it is not enough to apply the rules; you have to know which rules to select in the first place." (Erik J Larson, "The Myth of Artficial Intelligence: Why computers can't think the way we do", 2021)

"If chess permits a virtually infinite variety of games, the rules of nature  surely do. Science may be immortal after all." (John Horgan)

On Chess V: Chess and Mathematics III

"The game of chess has always fascinated mathematicians, and there is reason to suppose that the possession of great powers of playing that game is in many features very much like the possession of great mathematical ability. There are the different pieces to learn, the pawns, the knights, the bishops, the castles, and the queen and king. The board possesses certain possible combinations of squares, as in rows, diagonals, etc. The pieces are subject to certain rules by which their motions are governed, and there are other rules governing the players. [...] One has only to increase the number of pieces, to enlarge the field of the board, and to produce new rules which are to govern either the pieces or the player, to have a pretty good idea of what mathematics consists." (James B Shaw, "What is Mathematics?", Bulletin American Mathematical Society Vol. 18, 1912)

"Reductio ad absurdum, which Euclid loved so much, is one of a mathematician's finest weapons. It is a far finer gambit than any chess play: a chess player may offer the sacrifice of a pawn or even a piece, but a mathematician offers the game." (Godfrey H Hardy, "A Mathematician's Apology", 1940)

"Pure mathematics is the world's best game. It is more absorbing than chess, more of a gamble than poker, and lasts longer than Monopoly. It's free. It can be played anywhere - Archimedes did it in a bathtub." (Richard J Trudeau, "Dots and Lines", 1976)

"There is still a great deal of uncharted territory in the opening phase of the game. New ideas, new concepts, new plans in old and forgotten variations, there is still much to discover in the opening. The tactical patterns and strategic concepts of the middle game have been well mapped out by generations of Grandmasters, although there are occasional fresh twists. In the endgame, however, the plans and possibilities are open and known to all, an almost mathematical exercise. This isn’t to say that everything is predetermined. With flawless play from both sides, the endgame will advance toward a predictable conclusion. But since humans are flawed, damage can be inflicted or repaired. Even if one player is at a clear disadvantage, he may simply outplay his opponent." (Garry Kasparov, "How Life Imitates Chess", 2007)

"As art, chess speaks to us of the personal decisions that are made in the course of a game. Looking at this facet of the game, the essential protagonist is the aesthetic sense rather than the capacity for calculation, which thus moves us closer to the human dimension and farther from mathematical algorithms." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"For all their wealth of content, for all the sum of history and social institution invested in them, music, mathematics, and chess are resplendently useless (applied mathematics is a higher plumbing, a kind of music for the police band). They are metaphysically trivial, irresponsible. They refuse to relate outward, to take reality for arbiter. This is the source of their witchery." (George Steiner, George Steiner at The New Yorker, 2009)

"In emergent processes, the whole is greater than the sum of the parts. A mathematical phenomenon that appears in certain dynamic systems also occurs within biological systems, from molecular interactions within the cells to the cognitive processes that we use to move within society. [...] Emergent patterns of ideas, beauty, desires, or tragicomedy wait, ready to trap the next traveler in their complex domain of neatly patterned squares - the never-ending world of chess metaphors." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"The master of chess is deeply familiar with these patterns and knows very well the position that would be beneficial to reach. The rest is thinking in a logical way (calculating) about how each piece should be moved to reach the new pattern that has already taken shape in the chess player’s mind. This way of facing chess is closely related to the solving of theorems in mathematics. For example, a mathematician who wishes to prove an equation needs to imagine how the terms on each side of the equal sign can be manipulated so that one is reduced to the other. The enterprise is far from easy, to judge by the more than two hundred years that have been needed to solve theorems such as that of Fermat (z^n = x^n + y^n), using diverse tricks to prove the equation." (Diego Rasskin-Gutman, "Chess Metaphors: Artificial Intelligence and the Human Mind", 2009)

"Chess is a perfect arena for just such an exerted exploration of the possible. Its chequered sea is very deep indeed. The mathematics behind the game’s complexity are staggering. […] For all its immensity, chess is a finite game. It is therefore at least conceivable that a machine might one day be programmed with the knowledge, deep down in its nodes, of every possible sequence of moves for every possible game. No combination, however ingenious, would ever surprise it; every board position would be as familiar as a face." (Daniel Tammet, "Thinking in Numbers" , 2012) 

"Chess, with its straightforward rules and tiny Cartesian playing field, is a game tailor-made for computers." (John Horgan, "The End of Science", 2015)

On Chess IV: Trivia II

"Education in Chess has to be an education in independent thinking and judging. Chess must not be memorized […]" (Emanuel Lasker, "Lasker's Manual of Chess", 1925)

"Human affairs are like a chess game. Only those who do not take it seriously can be called good players." (Hong Zicheng, "A Chinese Garden of Serenity: Epigrams from the Ming Dynasty", 1959)

"Truth derives its strength not so much from itself as from the brilliant contrast it makes with what is only apparently true. This applies especially to Chess, where it is often found that the profoundest moves do not much startle the imagination." (Emanuel Lasker, "Common Sense in Chess", 1965)

"Chess problems demand from the composer the same virtues that characterize all worthwhile art: originality, invention, conciseness, harmony, complexity, and splendid insincerity." (Vladimir Nabokov, "Poems and problems", 1971)

"Clinging to any form of conservatism can be dangerous. Become too conservative and you are unprepared for surprises. You cannot depend on luck. Logic is blind and often knows only its own past. Logic is good for playing chess but is often too slow for the needs of survival." (Frank Herbert," Chapterhouse: Dune", 1986)

"Chess is infinite, and one has to make only one ill-considered move, and one's opponent's wildest dreams will become reality." (David Bronstein, "200 Open Games", 1991)

"Independence of thought is a most valuable quality in a chess-player, both at the board and when preparing for a game." (David Bronstein, "200 Open Games", 1991) 

"The laws of chess are as beautiful as those governing the universe - and as deadly." (Katherine NevilleA Calculated Risk, 1992)

"Chess reflects the real world in miniature. Endeavor, struggle, success, and defeat - they are part of each game ever played." (Bruce Pandolfini, "Pandolfini's Ultimate Guide to Chess", 2008)

"Chess is a game by its form, an art by its content and a science by the difficulty of gaining mastery in it. Chess can convey as much happiness as a good book or work of music can. However, it is necessary to learn to play well and only afterwards will one experience real delight." (Tigran Petrosian)


On Chess III: Chess and Mathematics II

"Observe, finally, that this induction is possible only if the same operation can be repeated indefinitely. That is why the theory of chess can never become a science: the different moves of the game do not resemble one another." (Henri Poincaré, "On the Nature of Mathematical Reasoning", 1894)

"Chess problems are the hymn-tunes of mathematics." (Godfrey H Hardy, "A Mathematician's Apology", 1940)

"I will say only that if a chess problem is, in the crude sense, 'useless', then that is equally true of most of the best mathematics; that very little of mathematics is useful practically, and that the little [that is] is comparatively dull." (Godfrey H Hardy, "A Mathematician's Apology", 1940)

"There are three intellectual pursuits, and, so far as I am aware, only three, in which human beings have performed major feats before the age of puberty. They are music, mathematics, and chess." (George Steiner, "Extraterritorial", 1971)

"Geniuses of certain kinds - mathematicians, chess players, computer programmers - seem, if not mad, at least lacking in the social skills most easily identified with sanity." (James Gleick, "Genius: the life and science of Richard Feynman", 1992)

"Chess is not Mathematics, where ten is always more than one; in chess the King with a pawn can beat opponent's King with all pieces if they are placed badly." (Ashot Nadanian, [Interview at S'pore Chess News], 2010)

"Often the key contribution of intuition is to make us aware of weak points in a problem, places where it may be vulnerable to attack. A mathematical proof is like a battle, or if you prefer a less warlike metaphor, a game of chess. Once a potential weak point has been identified, the mathematician’s technical grasp of the machinery of mathematics can be brought to bear to exploit it." (Ian Stewart, "Visions of Infinity", 2013)

"Chess is the art that expresses the science of logic." (Mikhail Botvinnik)

"Every good mathematician should also be a good chess player and vice versa." (Henri Poincaré)

"Mathematics, like chess, requires too direct and personal a confrontation to allow graceful defeat." (Alfred Adler)

09 March 2021

Joseph Weizenbaum - Collected Quotes

"A higher-level formal language is an abstract machine." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation", 1976)

"A theory is, of course, not merely any grammatically correct text that uses a set of terms somehow symbolically related to reality. It is a systematic aggregate of statements of laws. Its content, its very value as theory, lies at least as much in the structure of the interconnections that relate its laws to one another, as in the laws themselves." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

"Computers make possible an entirely new relationship between theories and models. I have already said that theories are texts. Texts are written in a language. Computer languages are languages too, and theories may be written in them. Indeed, for the present purpose we need not restrict our attention to machine languages or even to the kinds of 'higher-level' languages we have discussed. We may include all languages, specifically also natural languages, that computers may be able to interpret. The point is precisely that computers do interpret texts given to them, in other words, that texts determine computers' behavior. Theories written in the form of computer programs are ordinary theories as seen from one point of view." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

"Machines, when they operate properly, are not merely law abiding; they are embodiments of law. To say that a specific machine is 'operating properly' is to assert that it is an embodiment of a law we know and wish to apply. We expect an ordinary desk calculator, for example, to be an embodiment of the laws of arithmetic we all know." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

"Man is not a machine, [...] although man most certainly processes information, he does not necessarily process it in the way computers do. Computers and men are not species of the same genus. [...] No other organism, and certainly no computer, can be made to confront genuine human problems in human terms. [...] However much intelligence computers may attain, now or in the future, theirs must always be an intelligence alien to genuine human problems and concerns." (Joesph Weizenbaum, Computer Power and Human Reason: From Judgment to Calculation, 1976)

"Programming systems can, of course, be built without plan and without knowledge, let alone understanding, of the deep structural issues involved, just as houses, cities, systems of dams, and national economic policies can be similarly hacked together. As a system so constructed begins to get large, however, it also becomes increasingly unstable. When one of its subfunctions fails in an unanticipated way, it may be patched until the manifest trouble disappears. But since there is no general theory of the whole system, the system itself can be only a more or less chaotic aggregate of subsystems whose influence on one another's behavior is discoverable only piecemeal and by experiment. The hacker spends part of his time at the console piling new subsystems onto the structure he has already built - he calls them 'new features' - and the rest of his time in attempts to account for the way in which substructures already in place misbehave. That is what he and the computer converse about." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

"The aim of the model is of course not to reproduce reality in all its complexity. It is rather to capture in a vivid, often formal, way what is essential to understanding some aspect of its structure or behavior." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

"The computer programmer is a creator of universes for which he alone is the lawgiver. No playwright, no stage director, no emperor, however powerful, has ever exercised such absolute authority to arrange a stage or field of battle and to command such unswervingly dutiful actors or troops." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

"The connection between a model and a theory is that a model satisfies a theory; that is, a model obeys those laws of behavior that a corresponding theory explicitly states or which may be derived from it. [...] Computers make possible an entirely new relationship between theories and models. [...] A theory written in the form of a computer program is [...] both a theory and, when placed on a computer and run, a model to which the theory applies." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

"There is a distinction between physically embodied machines, whose ultimate function is to transduce energy or deliver power, and abstract machines. i.e., machines that exist only as ideas. The laws which the former embody must be a subset of the laws that govern the real world. The laws that govern the behavior of abstract machines are not necessarily so constrained. One may, for example, design an abstract machine whose internal signals are propagated among its components at speeds greater than the speed of light, in clear violation of physical law. The fact that such a machine cannot actually be built does not prohibit the exploration of its behavior." (Joseph Weizenbaum, "Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation" , 1976)

08 March 2021

On Machines XII (Mind vs. Machines IV)

"In other words then, if a machine is expected to be infallible, it cannot also be intelligent. There are several theorems which say almost exactly that. But these theorems say nothing about how much intelligence may be displayed if a machine makes no pretense at infallibility." (Alan M Turing, 1946)

"The brain has been compared to a digital computer because the neuron, like a switch or valve, either does or does not complete a circuit. But at that point the similarity ends. The switch in the digital computer is constant in its effect, and its effect is large in proportion to the total output of the machine. The effect produced by the neuron varies with its recovery from [the] refractory phase and with its metabolic state. The number of neurons involved in any action runs into millions so that the influence of any one is negligible. [...] Any cell in the system can be dispensed with. [...] The brain is an analogical machine, not digital. Analysis of the integrative activities will probably have to be in statistical terms. (Karl S Lashley, "The problem of serial order in behavior", 1951)

"Although it sounds implausible, it might turn out that above a certain level of complexity, a machine ceased to be predictable, even in principle, and started doing things on its own account, or, to use a very revealing phrase, it might begin to have a mind of its own." (John R Lucas, "Minds, Machines and Gödel", 1959)

"There are now machines in the world that think, that learn and create. Moreover, their ability to do these things is going to increase rapidly until - in the visible future - the range of problems they can handle will be coextensive with the range to which the human mind has been applied." (Allen Newell & Herbert A Simon, "Human problem solving", 1976)

"We can divide those who uphold the doctrine that men are machines, or a similar doctrine, into two categories: those who deny the existence of mental events, or personal experiences, or of consciousness; [...] and those who admit the existence of mental events, but assert that they are 'epiphenomena' - that everything can be explained without them, since the material world is causally closed." (Karl Popper & John Eccles, "The self and its brain", 1977)

"It is essential to realize that a computer is not a mere 'number cruncher', or supercalculating arithmetic machine, although this is how computers are commonly regarded by people having no familiarity with artificial intelligence. Computers do not crunch numbers; they manipulate symbols. [...] Digital computers originally developed with mathematical problems in mind, are in fact general purpose symbol manipulating machines." (Margaret A Boden, "Minds and mechanisms", 1981)

"What makes people smarter than machines? They certainly are not quicker or more precise. Yet people are far better at perceiving objects in natural scenes and noting their relations, at understanding language and retrieving contextually appropriate information from memory, at making plans and carrying out contextually appropriate actions, and at a wide range of other natural cognitive tasks. People are also far better at learning to do these things more accurately and fluently through processing experience." (James L McClelland et al, "The appeal of parallel distributed processing", 1986)

"A popular myth says that the invention of the computer diminishes our sense of ourselves, because it shows that rational thought is not special to human beings, but can be carried on by a mere machine. It is a short stop from there to the conclusion that intelligence is mechanical, which many people find to be an affront to all that is most precious and singular about their humanness." (Jeremy Campbell, "The improbable machine", 1989)

"Looking at ourselves from the computer viewpoint, we cannot avoid seeing that natural language is our most important 'programming language'. This means that a vast portion of our knowledge and activity is, for us, best communicated and understood in our natural language. [...] One could say that natural language was our first great original artifact and, since, as we increasingly realize, languages are machines, so natural language, with our brains to run it, was our primal invention of the universal computer. One could say this except for the sneaking suspicion that language isn’t something we invented but something we became, not something we constructed but something in which we created, and recreated, ourselves. (Justin Leiber, "Invitation to cognitive science", 1991)

"On the other hand, those who design and build computers know exactly how the machines are working down in the hidden depths of their semiconductors. Computers can be taken apart, scrutinized, and put back together. Their activities can be tracked, analyzed, measured, and thus clearly understood - which is far from possible with the brain. This gives rise to the tempting assumption on the part of the builders and designers that computers can tell us something about brains, indeed, that the computer can serve as a model of the mind, which then comes to be seen as some manner of information processing machine, and possibly not as good at the job as the machine." (Theodore Roszak, "The Cult of Information", 1994)

Science (From Fiction to Science-Fiction)

"Science, my boy, is composed of errors, but errors that it is right to make, for they lead step by step to the truth." (Jules Verne, "A Journey to the Centre of the Earth", 1864)

"A mind truly opened to what science has to teach must see that it is a little thing. [...] Pain is simply our intrinsic medical adviser to warn us and stimulate us." (Herbert G Wells, "The Island of Doctor Moreau", 1896)

"Science of to-day - the superstition of to-morrow. Science of to-morrow - superstition of to-day." (Charles Fort, "The Book of the Damned", 1919)

"The superstitions of today are the scientific facts of tomorrow." (Hamilton Deane & John L. Balderston, "Dracula", 1927)

"There are no enemies in science, professor, only phenomena to study." (Charles Lederer, "The Thing (from Another World)", 1951)

"Science explains the world, but only Art can reconcile us to it." (Stanislaw Lem, "King Globares and the Sages", 1965)

"Science offers a sounder basis on which to formulate systems of thought and ethics." (Michael Moorcock, "Behold the Man", 1967)

"Science has so accustomed us to devising and accepting theories to account for the facts we observe, however fantastic, that our minds must begin their manufacture before we are aware of it." (Gene Wolfe, "Seven American Nights" (1978)

"science: A way of finding things out and then making them work. Science explains what is happening around us the whole time." (Terry Pratchett, "Wings", 1990)

Reality (From Fiction to Science-Fiction)

"The horror of the Same Old Thing is [...] an endless source of heresies in religion, folly in counsel, infidelity in marriage, and inconstancy in friendship. The humans live in time, and experience reality successively. To experience much of it, therefore, they must experience many different things; in other words, they must experience change. And since they need change, the Enemy (being a hedonist at heart) has made change pleasurable to them." (C. S. Lewis, "The Screwtape Letters", 1942)

"It is the normal lot of people who must live this life [in space] to be - by terrestrial standards—insane. Insanity under such conditions is a useful and logical defense mechanism, an invaluable and salutary retreat from reality." (Charles L Harness, "The Paradox Men", 1949)

"It seemed as if the structure of reality trembled for an instant, and that behind the world of the senses he caught a glimpse of another and totally different universe [...]" (Arthur C Clarke, "The City and the Stars", 1956)

"No live organism can continue for long to exist sanely under conditions of absolute reality; even larks and katydids are supposed, by some, to dream." (Shirley Jackson, "The Haunting of Hill House", 1959)

"Reality? It is only the illusion we can agree upon." (James Gunn, "The Joy Makers", 1961)

"When dreams become more important than reality, you give up travel, building, creating." (Gene Roddenberry, "Star Trek" ["The Menagerie"], 1966)

"The whole of modern so-called existence is an attempt to deny reality insofar as it exists."John Brunner, "Stand on Zanzibar", 1968)

"Reality, to me, is not so much something that you perceive, but something you make." (Philip K Dick, "The Android and the Human", 1972)

"The theory changes the reality it describes." (Philip K Dick, "Flow My Tears the Policeman Said", 1974)

"We exist in time. Time is what binds molecules to make your brown eyes, your yellow hair, your thick fingers. Time changes the structures, alters hair or fingers, dims the eyes, immutably mutating reality. Time, itself unchanging, is the cosmic glue, the universal antisolvent that holds our worlds together." (Marta Randall, "Secret Rider", 1976)

"There was no substitute for reality; one should beware of imitations." (Arthur C Clarke, "The Fountains of Paradise", 1979)

"Reality is that which when you stop believing in it, it doesn’t go away." (Philip K Dick, "Valis", 1981)

"The basic tool for the manipulation of reality is the manipulation of words. If you can control the meaning of words, you can control the people who must use the words." (Philip K Dick, "How to Build a Universe That Doesn’t Fall Apart Two Days Later", 1985)

"It is now known to science that there are many more dimensions than the classical four. Scientists say that these don’t normally impinge on the world because the extra dimensions are very small and curve in on themselves, and that since reality is fractal most of it is tucked inside itself. This means either that the universe is more full of wonders than we can hope to understand or, more probably, that scientists make things up as they go along." (Terry Pratchett, Pyramids, 1989)

"It is always hard when reality intrudes on belief." (Alan D Foster, "Cyber Way", 1990)

"The dreams of people are in the machines, a planet network of active imaginations hooked into their made-up, make-believe worlds. Artificial reality is taking over; it has its own children." (Storm Constantine, "Immaculate", 1991)

On Machines IX (From Fiction to Science-Fiction)

"The humans have a curious force they call ambition. It drives them, and, through them, it drives us. This force which keeps them active, we lack. Perhaps, in time, we machines will acquire it." (John Wyndham, "The Lost Machine", 1932)

"There are so many disadvantages in human construction which do not occur in us machines. [...] Some little thing here or there breaks - they stop working and then, in a short time, they are decomposing. Had he been a machine, like myself, I could have mended him, replaced the broken parts and made him as good as new, but with these animal structures one is almost helpless." (John Wyndham, "The Lost Machine", 1932)

"The machine does not isolate man from the great problems of nature but plunges him more deeply into them." (Antoine de Saint-Exupéry, “Wind, Sand, and Stars, 1939) 

"There’s an affinity between men and the machines they make. They make them out of their own brains, really, a sort of mental conception and gestation, and the result responds to the mind that created them, and to all human minds that understand and manipulate them." (Catherine L Moore, "No Woman Born", 1944)

"The machine is only a tool after all, which can help humanity progress faster by taking some of the burdens of calculations and interpretations off its back. The task of the human brain remains what it has always been; that of discovering new data to be analyzed, and of devising new concepts to be tested." (Isaac Asimov, "I, Robot", 1950)

"Too darned good a machine can be a menace, not a help." (John W Campbell Jr, "Cloak of Aesir", 1951)

"When your life has depended for a long while upon machines—upon tubes and wires and gadgets of all kinds - you must come to trust these things as a part of yourself." (Michael Shaara, "The Holes", 1954)

"If a machine had broken down, it would have been quickly replaced. But who can replace a man?" (Brian W Aldiss, "Who Can Replace a Man?", 1958)

"The study of thinking machines teaches us more about the brain than we can learn by introspective methods. Western man is externalizing himself in the form of gadgets." (William S Burroughs, "Naked Lunch", 1959)

"That perfected machines may one day succeed us is, I remember, an extremely commonplace notion on Earth. It prevails not only among poets and romantics but in all classes of society. Perhaps it is because it is so widespread, born spontaneously in popular imagination, that it irritates scientific minds. Perhaps it is also for this very reason that it contains a germ of truth. Only a germ: Machines will always be machines; the most perfected robot, always a robot." (Pierre Boulle, "Planet of the Apes", 1963)

"Once men turned their thinking over to machines in the hope that this would set them free. But that only permitted other men with machines to enslave them." (Frank Herbert, "Dune", 1965)

"The machines didn’t tire and the medi-techs never made computational errors but both lacked an essential something. Something only one human being, no matter how inadequate, could give to another." (Leo P Kelley, "The Handyman", 1965)

"Thou shalt not make a machine in the likeness of a human mind." (Frank Herbert, "Dune", 1965)

"What do such machines really do? They increase the number of things we can do without thinking. Things we do without thinking-there’s the real danger." (Frank Herbert, "Dune", 1965)

"These machines had become old and worn-out, had begun making mistakes; therefore they began to seem almost human." (Philip K Dick & Ray Nelson, "The Ganymede Takeover", 1967)

"A humanoid robot is like any other machine; it can fluctuate between being a benefit and a hazard very rapidly." (Philip K Dick, "Do Androids Dream of Electric Sheep?", 1968)

"Someday, the real masters of space would be machines, not men - and he was neither. Already conscious of his destiny, he took a somber pride in his unique loneliness - the first immortal midway between two orders of creation.
He would, after all, be an ambassador; between the old and the new - between the creatures of carbon and the creatures of metal who must one day supersede them.
Both would have need of him in the troubled centuries that lay ahead." (Arthur C Clarke, "A Meeting with Medusa", 1971)

"Man has reached the stage where he evolves through his machines." (Gene Wolfe, "Alien Stones", 1972)

"There was no easy way to heaven, or nirvana, or whatever it was that the faithful sought. Merit was acquired solely by one’s own efforts, not with the aid of machines. An interesting doctrine, and one containing much truth; but there were also times when only machines could do the job." (Arthur C Clarke, "The Fountains of Paradise", 1979)

"The dreams of people are in the machines, a planet network of active imaginations hooked into their made-up, make-believe worlds. Artificial reality is taking over; it has its own children." (Storm Constantine, "Immaculate", 1991)

"Computers bootstrap their own offspring, grow so wise and incomprehensible that their communiqués assume the hallmarks of dementia: unfocused and irrelevant to the barely-intelligent creatures left behind. And when your surpassing creations find the answers you asked for, you can't understand their analysis and you can't verify their answers. You have to take their word on faith." (Peter Watts, "Blindsight", 2006)

"The architecture - the mind - is knitting together. It’s sentience. Vague sentience. All these years of formulating machines that know something, while the secret is to create machines that don’t know something." (Scott Hutchins,  "A Working Theory of Love", 2012)

"Artificial intelligence is a concept that obscures accountability. Our problem is not machines acting like humans - it's humans acting like machines." (John Twelve Hawks, "Spark", 2014)
Related Posts Plugin for WordPress, Blogger...

On Data: Longitudinal Data

  "Longitudinal data sets are comprised of repeated observations of an outcome and a set of covariates for each of many subjects. One o...